Hockett's Design Features of Language
Hockett's Design Features, identified by
linguist Charles F. Hockett in the 1960s, are a set of characteristics that
define human language and distinguish it from animal communication. Initially,
Hockett proposed 13 design features, suggesting that while primate
communication uses the first nine, the last four (displacement, productivity,
cultural transmission, and duality) are unique to humans. He later expanded the
list to include prevarication, reflexiveness, and learnability as uniquely
human traits, asserting that all human languages, even the most basic, possess
these 16 features.
Design Features of Language
Vocal-Auditory
Channel: This refers to the use of speaking and
hearing as the primary mode for human language. Hockett's original definition
didn't consider sign language, reflecting the prevailing ideology of orality at
the time. It has since been updated to include other channels like
tactile-visual or chemical-olfactory.
Broadcast
Transmission and Directional Reception: When humans
speak, sounds are transmitted in all directions, but listeners can identify the
sound's origin. Similarly, signers broadcast to anyone within sight, and
observers can see who is signing. This is common in both human and animal communication.
Transitoriness:
Also known as rapid fading, this refers to the
temporary nature of language. Language sounds exist briefly and disappear once
the speaker stops. This also applies to signs. Writing and Inka khipus
(knot-tying), however, are more permanent forms of communication.
Interchangeability:
Humans can both give and receive identical
linguistic signals, meaning they aren't limited in the types of messages they
can send or hear. For instance, anyone can say 'I am a boy,' regardless of
their actual gender. This differs from prevarication (lying); the key is that a
speaker can physically produce any message, regardless of its truth. Not all
species have this feature. Queen ants, for example, produce unique chemical
scents to communicate their status, which other ants cannot produce.
Complete
Feedback: Speakers can hear their own speech and
adjust it in real-time. Similarly, signers can see, feel, and control their
signing.
Specialization:
Linguistic signals are meant for communication, not
for other biological functions. Human speech or signing is generally
intentional. Dog panting is an example of non-specialized communication. While
it can communicate that the dog is hot or thirsty, the primary purpose is to
cool the dog off, making the communication secondary.
Semanticity:
Specific sound signals are directly linked to
certain meanings.
Arbitrariness:
Languages consist of both arbitrary and iconic
symbols. Iconicity in spoken languages includes onomatopoeia (e.g., 'murmur').
However, most symbols lack a logical connection between the sound and its
meaning. The word 'car,' for example, is arbitrary because it doesn't resemble
an actual car. This is further illustrated by different languages having
different names for the same object. Signed languages can be somewhat iconic
(e.g., 'house' in ASL), but many signs are still arbitrary.
Discreteness:
Linguistic representations can be broken down into
small, discrete units that combine in rule-governed ways. These units are
perceived categorically, not continuously. For example, the English plural
morpheme /s/ is added to nouns to mark number and is perceived categorically,
not by varying the loudness of the /s/.
Displacement:
Humans can discuss things that aren't physically
present or don't exist. They can talk about the past, future, hopes, and
dreams, not just the here and now. This feature distinguishes human language
from primate communication.
Productivity:
Language users can create and understand novel
utterances, producing an unlimited number of expressions. Grammatical
patterning also plays a role, facilitating language use and comprehension.
Language is dynamic, with new idioms constantly emerging and signal meanings
varying by context.
Traditional
Transmission: Also known as cultural transmission,
this suggests that while humans have innate language abilities, language is
learned socially after birth. This contrasts with Chomsky's Universal Grammar,
asserting that language is acquired through interaction with experienced users
like parents or caretakers. Language and culture are intertwined in this
process.
Duality
of Patterning: Meaningful messages are composed of
distinct, smaller meaningful units (words and morphemes), which are themselves
made up of distinct, smaller, meaningless units (phonemes).
Prevarication:
This is the ability to lie or deceive, allowing
humans to make false or meaningless statements. This is a key distinction
between human and animal communication, as animals cannot lie or invent
non-existent things.
Reflexiveness:
Humans can use language to discuss language itself.
This unique feature allows humans to describe language, discuss its structure,
and share ideas about it, unlike animal communication.
Learnability:
Language can be taught and learned. Speakers can
learn multiple languages, with young children acquiring language easily,
although acquisition becomes more difficult after a critical period.
Design Features in Animal Communication
Hockett differentiated language from
general communication, defining language as a communication system possessing
all the characteristics mentioned above. While many animals communicate, only
systems with all these features qualify as language. Some animal communication
systems exhibit a notable number of these design features.
Ants:
Ants use the chemical-olfactory channel, producing
pheromones to communicate various messages, such as food location, danger,
colony defense, and relocation. Queen ants use special pheromones to signal
their status and manage colony activities. Ants also engage in warfare,
displaying tactics that suggest intricate communication.
Birds:
Bird communication includes the vocal-auditory
channel, broadcast transmission/directional reception, rapid fading,
semanticity, and arbitrariness. Bird communication is divided into songs (used
to attract mates) and calls (used to alert others to food, danger, and
coordinate movement). Bird communication is both discrete and continuous, using
syntax to arrange songs with musical notes acting as phonemes. Birds have a
limited number of songs and unique dialects. Some species, like the Southern
Pied Blabber and the Japanese Tit, exhibit duality of patterning.
Honeybees:
Honeybees use the space-movement channel, employing
dances like the round dance, waggle dance, and transitional dance to
communicate food location. The waggle dance, for example, indicates the
direction and distance of food through specific movements. Honeybee dancing
demonstrates displacement, as bees can communicate about food sources over 100
meters away, even without being physically present.